Resumen
Dado que la imagen de destino es una construcción importante para la investigación turística, las ciudades y regiones intentan entender y desarrollar una imagen positiva para garantizar el número de visitantes y los ingresos del turismo. El objetivo principal de este artículo es desarrollar un modelo de evaluación de la imagen de destino mediante el uso de imágenes compartidas en redes sociales virtuales. Proponemos que la imagen de un destino se pueda analizar a través de la forma en que los turistas representan el destino en las redes sociales virtuales. Por lo tanto, desarrollamos una medida para evaluar la imagen de destino basada en imágenes publicadas en Instagram y desarrollamos un modelo para analizar la imagen de destino basada en imágenes compartidas en una red social virtual. Nuestro método fue compuesto por un análisis de 1500 imágenes de tres destinos importantes en Brasil (Foz do Iguaçú, Río de Janeiro y Salvador). Nuestros resultados contribuyen a la investigación turística en tanto indican que es posible determinar las características principales de un destino por las imágenes en las redes sociales virtuales, y proporcionan un modelo de cinco dimensiones para hacerlo. Este estudio contribuye a la investigación turística en dos espacios. Por un lado, proponemos un método para evaluar la imagen de destino basada en imágenes publicadas en las redes sociales en lugar de los cuestionarios habituales; esto puede ayudar a los académicos al proporcionar una forma alternativa de evaluar la imagen de destino sin recordar la conveniencia social y el sesgo de no respuesta. Y por otro lado, este estudio también resulta provechoso para los profesionales y las políticas públicas en turismo al mostrar qué características de una imagen de destino son más prominentes a la imagen de destino basada en redes sociales virtuales.
Background
The visual analyzer is the main sensory channel for humans that connects them with the environment. And therefore, loss of sight, especially in adulthood, is accompanied by emotional, motivational and social consequences that directly affect the psychophysiological state of the individual himself, his communication in society and, often, the social status of the subject [1]. In 90% of cases of vision loss, this condition is accompanied by depression due to disability [2], and in case of loss of sight of traumatic genesis, this process is further exacerbated by negative psychopathological manifestations, caused by the additional aggravating effect of posttraumatic stress on the state of mental health of a person [3,4,5,6,7]. The obvious relevance of the problem implies the involvement of various approaches to its solution, and the neurophysiological approach, as we hope, will allow us to identify new neurophysiological predictors for effective diagnosis and treatment of these disorders, on the one hand, and on the other hand - to elucidate the physiological mechanisms of these conditions. Earlier our group carried out neurophysiological studies of patients with nonpsychotic mental disorders due to a partial loss of sight owed to trauma [8]. However, later we assumed that the group of patients, that was studied by us is not homogeneous, which made us take a more differentiated approach. The results of these studies are presented in this paper.
Methods
Comprehensive studies were carried out on the basis of the department of eye trauma, as well as the office of urgent care for eye trauma during 2010-2013. Studies were conducted with the obligatory observance of the principles of bioethics and deontology. The volunteer patients were informed of the nature and purpose of the studies and consent for research was obtained from them. A randomized group of subjects was formed in the period after the ophthalmologic intervention and determination of the volume and prognosis of sight loss. A total of 600 people with partial loss of sight of traumatic genesis (PLVTG) underwent a screening examination. The manifestations of an acute stress reaction were recorded in all patients. Within one to three months after discharge from the hospital, when patients underwent MSE, we conducted an in-depth clinical-psychopathological examination of patients. As a result two groups of research were formed: the main group (MG) - 200 Patients who, after a traumatic event that caused a partial loss of sight, were diagnosed with nonpsychotic mental disorders (NPD), and a comparison group (CG) - 200 people whose mental state was consistent with the so-called "conditional norm". The criteria for exclusion from the study were lack of informed consent, history of mental and behavioral disorders, and the presence of severe somatic diseases, during which the patient's mental state may be affected.
Clinical-psychopathological research was conducted through an in-depth clinical standardized interview using ICD-10 diagnostic criteria. A subjective evaluation of the available clinical and psychopathological manifestations was carried out using the Beck depression inventory [9], diagnostic techniques for self-assessment of reactive (R) and personal (P) anxiety of Ch. D. Spielberger-Y. L. Khanin [10]. The analysis of objective manifestations of psychopathological symptoms was carried out using Hamilton’s clinical rating scales of depression (D) and anxiety (A) HDRS and NARS [11]. The nosological structure of diagnosed NPD was represented by mental and behavioral disorders of cluster F43 - "response to stress and adaptive disorders" (22%), adaptation disorders with a predominance of disturbances of other emotions F43.23 (14.5%); Post-traumatic stress disorder F43.1 (11.5%), adaptation disorder with mixed emotions and behavior disorder F43.25 (3.5%).
Two groups were formed to carry out electrophysiological studies to identify the characteristics of neurophysiological mechanisms of the development of NDP owed to PLVTG from the MG: asthenia and anxiety, which included 15 men aged 22 to 57 years, and a CG of 20 men of men of the same age. The asthenia group included patients with a clinical prevalence of asthenia, in whom asthenic-depressive and asthenic-hypochondriac syndromes were identified. The anxiety group included patients with a clinical prevalence of anxiety with anxiety-depressive, anxious-phobic and obsessive-phobic syndromes.
Electrophysiological studies were carried out using the diagnostic complex "Amplaid MK15" (Italy). To study brain electrical activity, the method of event-related evoked potentials (ERP) was used of auditory modality. Which was connected with the asthenia and anxiety features of the tested groups, that had a disruption of the function of the visual system. The subject was binaurally presented with a series of stimuli in the pseudo-random (with a predetermined probability of the appearance of significant stimuli) sequence – oddball paradigm. A significant stimulus - a tone with a frequency of 4000 Hz was presented with a probability of 20%, a background stimulus - tone with a frequency of 1000 Hz was presented with a probability of 80%. The intensity of the sounds was 100 dB above the threshold of audibility [12]. The interval between stimuli was 2 seconds. The subject was instructed to react to the appearance of a significant signal by pressing the button, background stimuli were ignored. When the button was pressed, registration of the induced brain electrical activity occurred, as well as the time and correctness of a simple sensorimotor reaction. The registration of the ERP was carried out from the head surface with standard electroencephalographic electrodes located in the 10/20 (Jasper) system at the points: Cz - active electrode, A1 + A2 - common refractive electrode, Fpz – grounding electrode. The averaging of evoked potential was carried out based on 100 records [13]. Elimination of artifacts, including eye movement artifacts, was hardware-based. The components of P1, N1, P2, N2, P3 and N4 were marked out on the ERP curve registered for a significant stimulus (Fig. 1). The positive component in the range of latent periods from 30 to 80 ms was regarded as P1, negativity from 80 to 140 ms - as N1, the positive component of P2 was differentiated in the time interval 120-200 ms, N2 - between 180 and 320 ms from the start of the scan, and the late positive component P3 was detected in the range of 270-550 ms, and the next maximum negative bias potential was considered as component N4. Statistical software package Statistica 5.5 (StatSoft, USA) was used for statistical processing of the obtained experimental results. When checking statistical hypotheses, the values of criteria with significance level p <0.05 were considered reliable. The comparative analysis was carried out by nonparametric methods: the Wilcoxon T-test and the Man-Whitney U test, and the Pearson-Spearman correlation analysis [14].
Figure 1.Acoustic ERP in the comparison group, in the main group and in asthenia group.*The main components of the curve (P1, N1, P2, N2, P3, N4), whose indices were analyzed, are indicated. Positive deviation of the potential goes down.
Results
The results of psychological testing in asthenia and anxiety groups
The results of psychological testing show the differences between the groups in all indicators: RA, PA, Ds, Do, Ao (Fig. 2). At the same time, the values of the three parameters - RA, PA and Do in the anxiety group were higher than in the asthenia group. Thus, the RA index in the anxiety group (59.3 ± 2.13) was higher (U [15] = 0; p <0.001) than in the asthenia group (49.7 ± 2.12). PA in the anxiety group (46.7 ± 3.81) also exceeded (U = 25, p <0.001) the values of the asthenia group (35.0 ± 6.92), and the Ao values in the anxiety group (22,0 ± 0.85) were almost twice as large (U = 0, p <0.001) than in the asthenia group (13.3 ± 2.97). The two remaining indicators: Ds (16.0 ± 0.84) and Do (17.7 ± 1.76) in the anxiety group were lower than in the asthenia group - 20.3 ± 2.58 (U = 12.5, p <0.001) and 24.67 ± 1.76 (U = 0; p <0.001) respectively.
Figure 2.The values of indicators of RA, PA, Ds, Do and Ao in the main asthenia and anxiety groups of subjects and in comparison group.* - differences with significance level of p<0,05. # - significant differences (p <0.05) in comparison with the asthenia group and * - with the comparison group.
The indices of psychological testing of asthenia and anxiety groups showed significant exceeding values (U values [15, 20] from 0 to 25, p <0.001) of RA, PA, Ds, Do and Ao of both groups relative towards the comparison group (Fig. 2).
ERP in the asthenia and anxiety groups
Comparative analysis of cognitive evoked potentials in anxiety and asthenia groups revealed a number of statistically significant differences (Fig. 3). The time of the sensorimotor reaction in the anxiety group was 337.3 ± 9.31 ms, which was significantly higher (U [15; 15] = 50; p <0.05) than in the asthenia group (308.7 ± 31.28 ms ). The latent period of the P1 component of the ERP in the anxiety group (76.0 ± 23.42 ms) significantly exceeded (U [15; 15] = 50, p <0.05) the latent period of this component in the asthenia group (52.3 ± 31 , 28 ms).
Figure 3.Time parameters of the ERP.*Time parameters of the ERP: time of sensorimotor reaction (T_ms), latent period of component P1, inter-peak intervals P1N1 and P3N4 in asthenia and anxiety groups. Medium values and standard deviations are presented. * - significant differences (p <0,05) in the studied groups.
The amplitude of the negative N2 component in the anxiety group (-6.54 ± 2.261 μV) had more negative values than in the asthenia group (-1.23 ± 1.644 μV) (U [15] = 0; p <0.001). The temporal indices of the ERP, such as the intervals P1N1 and P3N4, were smaler than in the asthenia group. Thus, the values of the P1N1 interval in the anxiety group were 31.0 ± 12.76 ms, and in the asthenia group they were 47.7 ± 17.94 ms (U [15, 15] = 50, p <0.05). And the interval of the later P3N4 complex in the anxiety group was longer than in the early complexes and equaled 126.0 ± 12.68 ms, which was less than in the asthenia group 139.0 ± 11.43 ms (U [15, 15 ] = 62.5, p <0.05).
ERP in asthenia and anxiety groups compared to the control group
Comparative analysis of the asthenia group with the comparison group revealed a sufficient number of indicators of the ERP, which have significant statistical differences. The correctness of the sensorimotor reaction in this group was 98.3 ± 2.44%, whereas in the CG - 92.5 ± 5.74% (U [15, 20] = 62.5, p <0.01). Also in this group there were differences in the amplitudes of the components: P1, N1, P2, N2 (Fig. 4,Fig. 5). So, the values of the amplitude of the early positivity of P1 in the asthenia group were 4.25 ± 3.312 μV, and in the CG -4.15 ± 7.933 μV (U [15, 20] = 50, p <0.001). The early negativity in that group was -2.78 ± 2.377 μV, and in the CG it was 10.55 ± 7.466 μV (U [15, 20] = 75; p <0.05). Differences between late P2 and N2 have the same trend of differences for the compared groups. The P2 values were 3.40 ± 2.029 μV in the asthenia group and -3.08 ± 8.287 μV in the CG (U [15, 20] = 75; p <0.05), the N2 value was -1.23 ± 1.644 μV , and in the CG it was 7.23 ± 5.754 μV (U [15, 20] = 75; p <0.05). In the anxiety group, there were other, different from the asthenia group, significant indicators of the ERP. Basically, these were time indicators, such as the latency period of the P1 component, which values in the anxiety group were 76.0 ± 23.42 ms, while in the CG they were 49.3 ± 4.38 ms (U [15, 20] = 0 ; P <0.001), as well as P1N1 intervals of 31.0 ± 12.76 ms in the anxiety group and 58.8 ± 7.447 ms in the CG (U [15, 20] = 0; p <0.001).
Figure 4.Amplitudes of positive components of ERP P1, P2, N1P2, P3N4 in the anxiety, asthenia and comparison groups.*significant differences (p <0.05) of the indicators compared to the comparison group
The interval of late N2P3 components in the anxiety group was 103.0 ± 24.09 ms, and in the CG it was 150.0 ± 48.812 ms (U [15; 20] = 75; p <0.05). In addition to the P1N1 interval in the anxiety group, significant differences from the CG showed the P1N1 amplitude swing, which was 31.0 ± 12.76 μV, whereas in the CG it was 6.39 ± 1.110 μV (U [15; 20] = 50; P <0.001). Among the indicators of the ERP were also those that had statistically significant differences from CG in both groups.
Figure 5.Amplitudes of negative components of ERP N1, N2, N4 in anxiety, asthenia and comparison groups.*significant differences (p <0.05) of the indicators compared to the comparison group.
These include such ERP indicators as latent periods of P3 and N4 components, amplitude N4 and amplitude ranges of N1P2 and P3N4. The latent periods of the P3 component and in the asthenia group are 312.0 ± 33.16 ms (U = 50; p <0.001), and in the anxiety group 320.0 ± 34.61 ms (U [15; 20] = 87.5, p <0.05) were lower than in the CG (368.3 ± 37.15 ms) (Fig. 6). The latent periods of component N4 and in the asthenia group (451.0 ± 33.09 ms, (U = 25; p <0.001)), and in the anxiety group (446.0 ± 25.52 ms, (U = 50; p <0.001)) were lower than the values of this component in the CG (498.8 ± 32.67 ms). Amplitudes of this component had more negative values: -6.36 ± 1.44 μV (U = 75; p <0.05) - in the asthenia group, -6.93 ± 4.379 μV (U = 50, p <0.001) - in the anxiety group compared to the CG, where its values were -0.29 ± 6.789 μV. If the amplitude of the N1P2 amplitudes in the asthenia group (6.19 ± 0.416 μV (U = 75; p <0.05)) and anxiety group (4.75 ± 2.538 μV (U = 50; P <0.001)) were smaller than the CG (7.47 ± 2.599 μV), when the amplitude of the later P3N4 was 18.69 ± 5.525 μV (U = 25; p <0.001) and 18.75 ± 0.873 ΜV (U = 75; p <0.05), respectively, exceeded the values of CG (11.04 ± 5.965 μV).
Figure 6. Figure 6. Time parameters of ERP: latent periods of P1, P3, N4 components and P1N1, N2P3 intervals in anxiety, asthenia and comparison groups.*significant differences (p <0.05) of the indicators compared to the comparison group.
Discussion
It should be noted that depressive and anxious manifestations were detected not only in anxiety and asthenia groups, formed as subgroups from the MG, but also among the comparison group. However, in the examined of the CG only a single symptom was detected that did not constitute a significant clinical picture of the mental disorder and had a low degree of severity, while in MG patients depressive and anxiety symptoms were diagnosed with a severity of small (72.5%) or severe (11.5%) depressive episode, as well as anxiety (56.0%) or anxiety disorder (32.5%). For persons with NPD, subjectively and objectively significant depressive manifestations were characteristic, primarily of the cognitive-affective and somatic spheres, as well as a high "dynamically stable" pathological anxiety with a negative drift of reactive anxiety in the direction of increase, on the background of the presence of a pathological basis in the form of the prevalence of a high level of personal anxiety. Among the respondents of the MG, on the contrary, according to their subjective assessment, the manifestations of depression were either absent or had a slight degree of severity, and present individual depressive symptoms characterized the process of the person's response to trauma. The assumption of heterogeneity of the MG was initially evaluated on the basis of available clinical and psychopathological manifestations, both subjective and objective (Figure 2). Both groups showed statistically significant differences, both among themselves and differences from the CG. And these differences were characterized as objective (Do and Ao), as well as subjective indicators (RA, PA and Ds).
Later we found that there are a number of differences in the nature of brain activity, which is manifested in the indicators of the ERP. Thus, higher values of the sensorimotor reaction time in the asthenia group in comparison with the anxiety group were combined with the higher values of the latency period of the P1 component, which points to a significant contribution to the process of implementing the sensorimotor response of early sensory perception mechanisms and attention mechanisms, and the whole brain activation level at the moment of the test [17]. In the anxiety group, this process was less rapid than in the asthenia group. The reason for this seems to be a decrease in the efficiency of the brake mechanisms of the sensor gates, which screen out irrelevant information projected through the medial geniculate bodies into the projection sections of the auditory cortex [18,19]. And the implementation of this mechanism, in the context of P1component, is associated with cholinergic [20,21] nonspecific brain systems. The interval between component P1 and the subsequent negative component N1 in the group was also increased in the asthenia group. Despite its polygenerational nature, [22] it is also associated with selective attention mechanisms [23], and among N1 generators such brain structures as the projection and associative regions of the cortex of the cerebral hemispheres, the nucleus of the medial and dorsal thalamus, the hippocampus, tonsils and structures of the reticular formation of the midbrain [24].
The P3N4 interval is formed by the two late ERP components - P3 and N4, that are polygenerator. The main P3 wave generators are localized in such structures of the central nervous system as the hippocampus, frontal and parietal parts of the cerebral cortex [25], as well as a number of subcortical structures and primarily the thalamus nucleus [26]. Neurons generating late negativity of N4 refer to novelty detectors that are activated with active, non-automatic recognition of deviations of audio signals from standard signals. The positive component of P3 is generated when any deviations of audio signals from standard signals are perceived [27]. Thus, the P3N4 complex can be interpreted as an electrophysiological correlate of the recognition of significant acoustic stimuli, and an increase in this interval in the asthenia group, in comparison with the anxiety group (Fig. 3), can be interpreted as an increase in the necessary time for the realization of this process.
Summarizing the results of a comparative analysis of the ERP in asthenia and anxiety group with CG, it should be noted that in these groups two types of indicators were identified. The first type is the components of the ERP, which have statistically significant differences from the CG in both the asthenia group and the anxiety group. These are the latent periods of the components P3 and N4, the amplitude N4, the amplitude swing N1P2 and P3N4. The second type is unique for each group of indicators of the ERP, distinguishing them from the CG.
In the asthenia group this is the correctness of the sensorimotor reaction and the amplitude of the components: P1, N1, P2, N2. In the anxiety group, such indicators were: latency period P1, intervals P1N1 and N2P3, amplitude swing P1N1. The N1P2 complex is part of the so-called V-wave (or complex P1-N1-P2), which, in fact, is the response of the auditory cortex to the stimulus [28,29,30]. Complex N1P2 is associated with a conscious distinction of any discrete changes in acoustic signals (tonality, loudness, modulation, duration, localization in space, etc.) [31]. It has multiple cortical and subcortical generators, the neurons of which enter the serotoninergic system of the brain, modulating the primary auditory cortex [32,33]. And a significant role in this is played by nonspecific brain stem systems [34]. The decrease in the amplitude swing of N1P2 in both the asthenia group and, to a greater extent, in the anxiety group, should be interpreted as a decrease in the modulating effects of the suture nuclei on the auditory cortex. The effect of decreasing of the amplitude of N1P2 was observed in the experiment with the action of sedatives on healthy subjects, and in the phase of deep sleep it increased [35]. And as it is known, it is during deep sleep that the activity of serotoninergic neurons of the seam nuclei increases. With the amplitude of the late complex P3N4 in the asthenia and anxiety groups, we observe the opposite effect. It exceeds the values of the CG, which can be interpreted as a compensatory mechanism of active recognition, implemented in the late stages of the cognitive process, which allows successful solving the experimental task, even with the reduced effectiveness of early recognition mechanisms. This mechanism probably can also explain the decrease in the latent periods of components P3 and N4 in asthenia and anxiety group.
A specific marker of the asthenia group, distinguishing it from the CG, was the more positive values of the amplitude of the components P1, N1, P2, N2. Taking into account the low-frequency nature of the modulation of the amplitudes of these components (circa 2 Hz), it can be assumed that nonspecific brainstem systems are involved in the process.
In the anxiety group the most significant neurophysiological markers were: the latency period P1, the interval and amplitude range of the P1N1 complex. A significant increase in the latency period P1, which resulted in the shortening of the P1N1 interval and a decrease of the amplitude of this complex, should be interpreted as a decrease in the effectiveness of early selective attention mechanisms that ensure the screening out of relevant information and implemented largely by cholinergic mediator systems. Despite this, the implementation of the sensorimotor reaction in this group remained at the level of the CG values, which ensured the activation of compensatory mechanisms at the later stages of sensory information processing. The N2P3 interval correlates with these processes, which became much shorter in the anxiety group than in the CG. It is known that the N2P3 complex occurs only when the sound stimuli are identified, and not only heard [35]. The distribution of the complex on the scalp points to the generators located in the frontal and anterior temporal region of the cortex of the cerebral hemispheres, which is associated with extensive activation of these areas of the brain in the process of selective attention.
Conclusion
- Neurophysiological heterogeneity of a group of patients with nonpsychotic mental disorders was revealed due to a partial loss of sight of traumatic genesis.
- Unique neurophysiologic markers of asthenia and anxiety groups distinguish these groups from the CG.
- Neurophysiological markers have been identified that unite asthenia groups and control in a group of patients with nonpsychotic mental disorders due to partial loss of sight of traumatic genesis.
- Possible neurophysiological mechanisms of the revealed changes in the induced electrical activity of the brain in the study groups are discussed.
References
- Loprinzi PD, Smit E, Pariser G. Association Among Depression, Physical Functioning, and Hearing and Vision Impairment in Adults With Diabetes. Diabetes Spectrum. 2013; 26(1):6-15. DOI
- Rovner BW, Casten RJ. Activity loss and depression in age-related macular de-generation. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002; 10(3):305-310. PubMed
- Leo D, Hickey PA, Meneghel G, Cantor CH. Blindness, Fear of Sight Loss, and Suicide. Psychosomatics. 1999; 40:339-344. DOI
- Hine TJ, Pitchford NJ, Kingdom FAA, Koenekoop R. Blindness and High Suicide Risk?. Psychosomatics. 2000; 41(4):370-371. DOI
- Feu M, Fergusson K. Sensory impairment and mental health. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment. 2003; 9(2):95-103. DOI
- Thurston M. An inquiry into the emotional impact of sight loss and the counselling experiences and needs of blind and partially sighted people. Counselling and Psychotherapy Research: Linking research with practice. 2010; 10(1):3-12. DOI
- Rees G, Tee HW, Marella M. Vision-Specific Distress and Depressive Symptoms in People with Vision Impairment. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010; 51(6):2891-2896. DOI
- Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An Inventory for Measuring Depression. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1961; 4:8-18. PubMed
- Raygorodskiy DY. Prakticheskaya psihodiagnostika: metodiki i testyi. Bahrah: S-P; 2002.
- Podkoryitov VS, Chayka YuYu. Depressii (Sovremennaya terapiya). Tornado: Harkov; 2003.
- Goodin D, Desmedt J, Maurer K, Nuwer MR. IFCN recommended standards for long latency auditory event-related potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994; 91(1):18-20. PubMed
- Polich J, Ellerson PC, Cohen J. P300, stimulus intensity, modality, and probability. Int J Psychophysiol. 1996; 23(1):55-62. PubMed
- Kendal M, Styuart A. Statisticheskie vyivodyi i svyazi. Nauka: Moskva; 1973.
- Borovikov VP, Borovikov IP. STATISTICA - Statisticheskiy analiz i obrabotka dannyih v srede Windows. Informatsionno-izdatelskiy dom “Filin”: Moskva; 1997.
- Glants S. Mediko-biologicheskaya statistika. Praktika: Moskva; 1998.
- Schnyer DM, Allen JJ. Attention-related electroencephalographic and event-related potential predictors of responsiveness to suggested posthypnotic amnesia. Int J Clin Exp Hypn. 1995; 43(3):295-315.
- Buchwald JS, Erwin R, Lancker D, Guthrie D, Schwafel J, Tanguay P. Midlatency auditory evoked responses: P1 abnormalities in adult autistic subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1992; 84:164-171. DOI
- Patterson JV, Jin Y, Gierczak M, Hetrick WP, Potkin S, Bunney WE, Sandman CA. Effects of temporal variability on p50 and the gating ratio in schizophrenia: A frequency domain adaptive filter single-trial analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000; 57:57-64. PubMed
- Green JB, Burba A, Freed DM. The P1 component of the middle latency auditory potential may differentiate a brainstem subgroup of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997; 11(3):153-157. PubMed
- Bushwald JS, Rubinstein EN, Schwafel J, Strandburg RJ. Midlatency auditory evoked responses: differential effects of cholinergic agonist and antagonist. EEG and Clin Neurophysiol. 1991; 80(4):303-312. PubMed
- Naatanen R. Vnimanie i funktsii mozga: Ucheb. Izd-vo MGU: Posobie. Moskva; 1998.
- Vogel EK, Luck SJ. The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology. 2000; 37:190-203. PubMed
- Velasco M, Velasco F. Subcortical correlates of the somatic, auditory and visual vertex activites in man. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1985; 61(6):519-529. PubMed
- Polich J, Alexander JE, Bauer LO. P300 topography of amplitude/latency correlations. Brain Topogr. 1997; 9(4):275-282. PubMed
- Kropotov JD, Naatnen R, Sevostianov AV. Mismatch negativity to auditory stimulus change recorded directly from the human temporal cortex. Psychophysiology. 1995; 32(4):418-422. PubMed
- McCarthy G, Nobre AC. Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993; 88(3):210-219. DOI
- Gnezditskiy VV. Vyizvannyie potentsialyi mozga v klinicheskoy praktike. Izd-vo TRTU: Taganrog; 1997.
- Donchin E, Coles MGH. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?. The bichavioral and Brain Sciences. 1988; 11:357-374. DOI
- McCarthy G, Nobre AC. Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993; 88(3):210-219. PubMed
- Uhlen IT, Borg E, Persson HE, Spens KE. Topography of auditory evoked cortical potentials in children with severe language impairment: the N1 component. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996; 100(3):250-260. PubMed
- Hyde M. The N1 response and its applications. Audiol Neurootol. 1997; 2(5):281-307. PubMed
- Juckel G, Molnar M, Hegerl U. Auditory-evoked potentials as indicator of brain serotonergic activity—first evidence in behaving cats. Biol Psychiatry. 1997; 41(12):1181-1195. DOI
- Kevanishvili ZS, Davitashvili OZ. Korotko- sredne- i dlinnolatentnyie sluhovyie vyizvannyie potentsialyi pri mono- i binauralnom zvukovom razdrazhenii. Paper presented at: Korotko- sredne- i dlinnolatentnyie sluhovyie vyizvannyie potentsialyi pri mono- i binauralnom zvukovom razdrazhenii; 1983;
- Arehole S. A preliminary study of the relationship between long latency response and learning disorder. Br J Audiol. 1995; 29(6):295-298. PubMed
- Potts GF, Dien J, Hartry-Speiser AL. Dense sensor array topography of the event-related potential to task-relevant auditory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998; 106(5):444-456. PubMed